[-] Show simple item record

dc.contributor.advisorWood, Phillip K. (Phillip Karl)eng
dc.contributor.authorWilliams, Michelle A., 1980-eng
dc.date.issued2010eng
dc.date.submitted2010 Springeng
dc.descriptionTitle from PDF of title page (University of Missouri--Columbia, viewed on May 28, 2010).eng
dc.descriptionThe entire thesis text is included in the research.pdf file; the official abstract appears in the short.pdf file; a non-technical public abstract appears in the public.pdf file.eng
dc.descriptionDissertation advisor: Dr. Phillip Wood.eng
dc.descriptionVita.eng
dc.descriptionPh. D. University of Missouri--Columbia 2010.eng
dc.description.abstractChange over time can be modeled through a variety of statistical techniques including, but not limited to, linear or quadratic slope-intercept (SI), repeated-measures MANOVA, and single-factor growth models. Choice of method for modeling longitudinal data is often based on convention or familiarity with a particular modeling approach. It is argued that this should not be common practice, but rather the data and model comparisons should be used to inform the choice of model. The current work shows how several growth models are actually special cases of the free curve slope-intercept (FCSI) model, and as such, the models are nested within the FCSI model. Given the nested structure, model comparisons can be made using chi-square difference tests as well as comparisons of alternative fit indices (e.g., AIC, BIC, RMSEA) to determine the best model for a given dataset. This idea is illustrated through application to an example dataset that included longitudinal measurements of cognitive ability information over adulthood. Substantively, the results suggest that crystallized abilities change at a non-linear rate that is not adequately characterized by the commonly used quadratic or linear slope-intercept model. Specifically, the FCSI model was indicated as appropriate for all crystallized abilities. Fluid abilities, however, showed a more constant rate of change than crystallized abilities, consistent with the literature. More specifically, the three fluid abilities were adequately modeled by a linear SI, a quadratic SI, and a single-factor growth model that showed a linear tendency.eng
dc.description.bibrefIncludes bibliographical references.eng
dc.format.extentviii, 98 pageseng
dc.identifier.merlinb77846783eng
dc.identifier.oclc656875759eng
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10355/8301
dc.identifier.urihttps://doi.org/10.32469/10355/8301eng
dc.languageEnglisheng
dc.publisherUniversity of Missouri--Columbiaeng
dc.relation.ispartofcommunityUniversity of Missouri--Columbia. Graduate School. Theses and Dissertationseng
dc.rightsOpenAccess.eng
dc.rights.licenseThis work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs 3.0 License.
dc.subject.lcshCognition -- Testingeng
dc.subject.lcshCognition -- Age factorseng
dc.subject.lcshPsychometricseng
dc.titleUsing nested structures to select models for developmental trajectories of cognitive abilities in adulthoodeng
dc.typeThesiseng
thesis.degree.disciplinePsychological sciences (MU)eng
thesis.degree.grantorUniversity of Missouri--Columbiaeng
thesis.degree.levelDoctoraleng
thesis.degree.namePh. D.eng


Files in this item

[PDF]
[PDF]
[PDF]

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

[-] Show simple item record